Skip to content →

The Power of Deliberate Practice Part 3: Experience, Feedback, Brain Changes, Motivation, and Persistence

Last updated on December 16, 2024

When you think of a concert pianist, an Olympic gold medalist, or a world-renowned surgeon, what comes to mind? Is it raw, unfiltered talent? A gift bestowed upon a select few? Or maybe, it’s the hours of sweat, sacrifice, and solitary focus that turned that gift into greatness.

If you’re like most of us, you’ve probably thought that these people were just born different. Special. But what if I told you that the true secret to their success isn’t talent at all? What if it’s something you could do, too? This is where the concept of deliberate practice comes in—a concept that revolutionized how we understand expertise and peak performance.

Today, we’re diving into the heart of what makes experts stand apart. We’ll explore 3 cornerstone papers, focusing on the 1996 research that takes everything we thought we knew about mastery and elevates it. By the end, you’ll see how these insights can guide your personal growth, help you improve faster, and maybe even convince you that greatness isn’t reserved for the few—it’s within reach for anyone willing to put in the work.

Deliberate Practice, the Beginning: K. Anders Ericsson’s 1993 and 1994 Papers

Let me take you back to 1993. Picture a group of violinists gathered for a study. Some are world-class performers, others are teachers, and still others are students hoping to someday reach the top. Researchers are curious: What sets these world-class performers apart from the rest?

The answer wasn’t talent or even time spent playing. What the researchers found was something much more profound: It wasn’t the amount of time the violinists spent practicing. It was how they practiced that made all the difference. This was the origin of the concept of deliberate practice.

You see, unlike casual practice, deliberate practice is hard. It’s focused. It’s the kind of practice where you push your limits, make mistakes, and seek out feedback so you can get better. Here’s the kicker: it’s not always fun. In fact, it’s often mentally exhausting. It’s the kind of practice that leads to mastery, though.

Fast forward to 1994. Now, the researchers were looking beyond just physical skills—they wanted to understand how cognitive skills develop through deliberate practice. They studied chess players and doctors, discovering that experts don’t just practice more; they practice smarter. They build mental structures that allow them to solve problems faster and more accurately than the rest of us. A chess grandmaster, for example, doesn’t just see pieces on a board; they see patterns, strategies, and possibilities in ways that amateurs can’t.

So by the mid-90s, many were of the mind that deliberate practice is key to becoming the best. Then came the 1996 paper by Ericsson and Lehmann—a study that not only advanced these findings, it went deeper, challenging the certain assumptions experts held about expertise.

Ericsson’s 1996 Paper: Redefining Expert Performance

Now, let me take you on a different journey—a journey into the minds of the greatest performers the world has ever seen. The 1996 paper by Ericsson and Lehmann didn’t just build on the research of the past; it questioned some of our most deeply held beliefs about expertise.

1. More Experience Doesn’t Mean Better Performance

Picture this: Two artists, both with 20 years of experience. One has been drawing casually for two decades, sketching the same subjects over and over again. The other? He’s been pushing his limits every single day—studying anatomy, working with new materials, and seeking feedback from other artists.

Who do you think is better?

According to the 1996 paper, it’s not the one with the most experience—it’s the one who practiced deliberately. This was a huge shift in how we thought about performance. We often believe that if we just keep doing something long enough, we’ll eventually become great at it. Ericsson’s 1996 paper argues that’s simply not true.

Experience alone does not create expertise. Only focused, goal-oriented, structured practice does.

This insight hits home for so many of us. How often have you heard the phrase, “Practice makes perfect”? Well, the truth is, it doesn’t—deliberate practice makes perfect. Without deliberately seeking to improve, getting feedback, and stepping outside our comfort zones, we’ll just be spinning our wheels, no matter how long we practice.

2. The Power of Feedback: The Right Kind Changes Everything

Now, let me tell you a story about feedback.

Imagine a young basketball player, shooting hoops at the local gym. He’s been at it for hours—his form is improving, his shots are getting better, but something’s still off. Now, imagine a coach steps in, watches for a minute, and tells him, “You’re releasing the ball a fraction too late.”

In that moment, with that single piece of feedback, everything changes. The next shot? Swish.

The 1996 paper dives deep into the role of feedback in deliberate practice, showing that experts are constantly seeking feedback to refine their skills. Not just any feedback, mind you—it has to be specific, targeted, and immediate. That’s the difference between improving slowly and improving exponentially.

Without feedback, even deliberate practice can stall. It’s like trying to drive to a new destination without a GPS—you can keep driving, but without directions, how will you know if you’re on the right path?

3. Motivation: The Unseen Driver of Mastery

You’ve probably heard stories of Olympic athletes waking up at 4 AM to train, even when they don’t feel like it. What drives someone to do that? Motivation.

The 1996 paper reveals that motivation is a critical component of deliberate practice. Without the right mindset and drive, even the best-laid practice plans can fall flat. Think about it—how many of us have tried to learn a new skill, only to give up when it got hard or when we didn’t see immediate results? The best performers, whether in sports, business, or art, keep going, fueled by a desire to improve, not just to perform.

This relentless drive to improve is what separates the good from the great. It’s the reason why Kobe Bryant would shoot hundreds of free throws after games, or why Jeff Bezos would dig deep into customer feedback to make Amazon better.

4. Cognitive Adaptations: How Practice Changes the Brain

Let’s switch gears for a moment and talk about what happens inside the brain when you engage in deliberate practice.

The 1996 paper shines a light on the fascinating world of cognitive adaptations. Imagine a doctor diagnosing a complex case in minutes, while another doctor struggles to figure it out after hours of examination. The difference isn’t just experience—it’s how their brains have adapted over years of deliberate practice.

The brains of experts change in ways that allow them to process information faster and more efficiently. This is why a chess grandmaster can anticipate several moves ahead, or why a seasoned musician can play complex pieces from memory. Their brains have adapted to store and retrieve domain-specific information in ways that make their performance almost automatic.

Here’s the catch: These adaptations are domain-specific. Just because someone is an expert in one area doesn’t mean they’ll be an expert in another. A chess grandmaster’s mental prowess won’t necessarily help them play the violin. Expertise is built through deliberate practice in a specific field—it doesn’t magically transfer to everything else.

5. The 10-Year Rule: Patience, Persistence, and the Long Game

Let me tell you a story about a young tennis player. She’s good—really good. She’s been playing since she was five years old, practicing with a coach, competing in local tournaments. By the time she’s 15, she’s winning championships. Then something happens. She hits a plateau. No matter how hard she practices, she can’t seem to get better.

That’s where the 10-year rule comes in.

The 1996 paper reinforces what’s known as the 10-year rulethe idea that it takes about a decade of deliberate practice to reach expert-level performance. Here’s the twist: not everyone who practices for 10 years will become an expert.

Why? Because it’s not just about putting in the time—it’s about how you spend that time. Only those who engage in deliberate practice, seek feedback, and stay motivated will reach the top. This rule applies across domains—whether you’re a musician, an athlete, or a business leader, it takes years of dedicated effort to reach the highest levels of performance.

Insights and Takeaways

After diving deep into the research, here are 4 thoughts that stand out:

It’s not about time, it’s about quality.

Whether you’re practicing a sport, honing a business skill, or learning a new craft, focus on how you practice. Deliberate, structured, and goal-oriented practice leads to success—not just putting in the hours.

Motivation fuels mastery.

Without the right mindset and motivation, even the best practice won’t take you far. Cultivate a drive that pushes you beyond your comfort zone, and success will follow.

The most fun practice is often the least effective.

We tend to believe that if something is enjoyable, it’s beneficial. But deliberate practice tells us otherwise. The most productive practice is often the least enjoyable. In fact, it’s during the tough, uncomfortable, and mentally draining sessions that real growth happens. The moments when we’re pushed to our limits are the moments that drive improvement.

Instant success doesn’t last.

In a world that celebrates overnight success, the 1996 paper reminds us that true, lasting success is built over time. Quick wins might feel satisfying in the moment, but they rarely lead to sustained excellence. The stories of people who rise quickly and fade just as fast are proof of this. The real magic happens in the slow, steady grind of deliberate practice.

In the end, Ericsson’s 1996 paper deepens our understanding of expertise and shows us that greatness isn’t a gift—it’s earned through hard, focused, and deliberate work.


The papers discussed in this post are:

Ericsson, K. A., Jr., Krampe, R. Th., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review (Vol. 100, Issue 3, pp. 363–406). The paper may be found here.

Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist49(8), 725–747. The paper may be found here.

Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints (Feb. 1996), Annual Review of Psychology (47(1):273-305 47(1):273-305). The paper may be found here.

Published in Deliberate Practice

Comments

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *