Skip to content →

The Power of Deliberate Practice Part 2: When Should You Start Deliberate Practice, and How Should You Do It?

Last updated on December 16, 2024

Imagine you’re watching a world-class violinist perform, or perhaps you’re awed by an elite chess player’s ability to see 15 moves ahead. The natural reaction is to think, “They must be born with a gift.” What if I told you that their expertise wasn’t gifted by the gods or predetermined by some innate talent? Instead, their mastery was molded by something available to us all: deliberate practice.

In the 1993 paper “The Role of Deliberate Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance” and its follow-up in 1994, K. Anders Ericsson and Neil Charness dive into the game-changing concept of deliberate practice. While the 1993 paper introduces deliberate practice as the secret sauce to achieving expert-level skills, the 1994 paper goes further. It refines the framework, adds new insights, and challenges many long-held assumptions about talent, growth, and performance.

Today, we’ll explore the fascinating conversation between these two papers. I’ll walk you through what the 1994 paper adds to the 1993 one, what’s different, and the key findings that can forever change how you approach personal growth, continuous improvement, and peak performance.

Ericsson’s 1993 Paper on Deliberate Practice: A New Paradigm

To understand what the 1994 paper brings to the table, we must first delve into the essence of the 1993 paper.

The 1993 paper revolutionizes the way we think about becoming an expert. It introduces deliberate practice as a structured and purposeful activity designed solely to improve performance. This is not the same as playing for fun or simply “working hard.” Instead, it involves targeted, purposeful practice with feedback, continual refinement, and pushing limits.

Ericsson and his team presented a bold claim: The road to expertise isn’t talent-driven. Instead, expert performers in fields like chess, music, and sports achieve greatness by engaging in high levels of deliberate practice over many years, often beginning at a young age. The researchers argue that talent plays a far smaller role than once believed, and deliberate practice is the real differentiator.

Data from chess grandmasters, professional musicians, and athletes support this theory. All these experts accumulated thousands of hours of deliberate practice, spanning over a decade. The study challenges the idea that there is a fixed ceiling to human potential, suggesting that, given the right practice, most individuals can reach levels previously thought to be unattainable.

1994: Expanding the Concept of Deliberate Practice

If the 1993 paper introduced us to the theory, the 1994 paper expanded and enriched it. Here’s how the 1994 paper refines and adds new layers to the earlier study.

1. The Structure of Expert Performance

In 1994, Ericsson and Charness go beyond simply discussing how deliberate practice works. They explore the actual structure of expert performance. The paper emphasizes that expert performance is more than an incremental refinement of skills. It’s about acquiring entirely new methods and ways of thinking that circumvent the usual limits of human performance.

For instance, elite chess players don’t just memorize positions; they develop complex internal structures that help them anticipate several moves ahead. Musicians similarly adapt by developing cognitive frameworks that allow them to interpret and process music more efficiently than novices. This goes beyond just doing more of the same practice – it’s about reorganizing the way they think.

The 1994 paper discusses how physiological changes also play a role. Through sustained, focused practice, athletes change both their skills and their bodies. Muscle fibers adapt, heart and lung capacity increases, and even brain structure shifts to accommodate these new demands. This is an exciting addition to the 1993 findings: deliberate practice doesn’t just enhance skills. It can actually rewire your body and brain for elite performance.

2. Age and the Role of Early Start

One of the most striking additions in the 1994 paper is the importance of starting early. While the 1993 paper touched on the long-term nature of deliberate practice, the 1994 study delves deeper into the relationship between early age training and eventual peak performance.

What the data reveals is that the sooner individuals begin their deliberate practice journey, the higher they tend to climb in their fields. Ericsson and Charness analyze graphs and empirical evidence, showing that starting before the age of 10 is often a crucial factor in reaching the highest levels of mastery. This early exposure allows children to gain a head start in the demanding journey of deliberate practice, which usually requires around 10 years or 10,000 hours of intense training to yield expert results.

The implications are significant: starting young provides a critical foundation for pushing the boundaries of human performance. What’s more fascinating is that deliberate practice still works for adults, though the timeline for mastering a skill may lengthen.

3. Individual Differences in Performance

While the 1993 paper frames deliberate practice as the primary pathway to expertise, the 1994 paper introduces additional nuance by discussing individual differences. Not everyone improves at the same rate, even with the same practice regimen.

This is where the 1994 paper makes a crucial distinction. Although everyone can improve through deliberate practice, the way individuals accumulate and apply that practice varies. Some people may respond better to feedback, while others may have higher levels of intrinsic motivation that drive them to push through plateaus. These individual factors account for performance differences, even among experts with similar practice hours.

Here’s where it gets even more interesting: motivation and mental resilience are often the key factors that differentiate the good from the truly great. It’s not just about practicing more, but about practicing smarter, consistently working on weaknesses, and maintaining a growth mindset over the years.

Teaching Methods and Supervision: The Role of Coaches

One of the most exciting additions in the 1994 paper is the emphasis on the role of teachers, coaches, and structured environments in driving expert performance. It’s not enough to simply practice on your own. Supervised deliberate practice, where experts give feedback, correct mistakes, and push individuals beyond their comfort zones, leads to far greater results than self-directed learning.

Ericsson and Charness offer historical examples to back this up. They highlight how master coaches and teachers in chess, sports, and music have guided their pupils through the grueling process of deliberate practice, helping them achieve elite status.

For example, the 1994 paper discusses how chess players improve by studying master-level games and attempting to predict moves before checking the actual results. This kind of structured self-study is akin to having a coach constantly pushing you to think ahead and refine your strategies.

What’s key here is that feedback and structured repetition are the ingredients that turn practice into something transformative. Without these elements, practice can become stagnant, and improvements plateau.

What the 1994 Paper Adds to the Knowledge Base about Deliberate Practice

While much of the 1994 paper builds on the findings from 1993, there are some entirely new insights worth highlighting:

1. The Role of Cognitive Restructuring

The 1994 paper emphasizes that experts don’t simply refine existing skills. Instead, they undergo a process of cognitive restructuring, which fundamentally changes the way they think, process information, and solve problems. This restructuring is what separates a true expert from a novice – it’s not just about accumulating knowledge. It’s about organizing accumulated knowledge it in a way that allows for rapid retrieval and application under pressure.

For instance, elite musicians don’t just memorize notes – they develop frameworks for interpreting and applying musical knowledge intuitively. Similarly, chess players build complex mental maps of the chessboard, allowing them to predict the outcomes of different moves faster than a novice could even think about them.

2. Physiological Adaptations

Another fresh perspective in the 1994 paper is the idea that deliberate practice doesn’t just change your mind; it changes your body. Athletes, for instance, experience profound changes in their muscle structure, heart capacity, and even their neural pathways through years of focused training.

These adaptations make it easier for experts to perform at a high level for extended periods. Whether it’s the increased lung capacity of a marathon runner or the finely tuned muscle memory of a violinist, these changes show that expert performance is more than just a mental game – it’s a full-body transformation.

Valuable Insights from Ericsson’s 1993 and 1994 Papers

The conversation between the 1993 and 1994 papers offers two valuable takeaways:

1. You are not limited by talent.

The biggest insight is that talent plays a far smaller role than once thought. What truly matters is the dedication and structure of your practice. The deliberate practice model shows that with focused, purposeful practice over time, nearly anyone can reach levels of expertise once thought unattainable.

2. It’s important to start early and persist.

While starting young can give you a leg up, adults should not feel discouraged. Deliberate practice works at any age, and even those who begin later in life can achieve remarkable levels of skill if they apply themselves correctly.

3. Deliberate practice is not inherently enjoyable.

We often hear that passion drives performance. Yet, this research reveals that the most effective practice is far from enjoyable. Elite performers spend hours on focused, often repetitive tasks that stretch them beyond their current capabilities. The pursuit of fun and flow might actually hold you back from improving.

4. More experience doesn’t mean better performance.

Contrary to popular belief, just doing more isn’t enough. Working hard and long without structured feedback, focused effort, or challenging your weaknesses leads to little improvement. In fact, without the deliberate nature of practice, performance can plateau early, leaving years of experience behind but no significant growth.

By applying these concepts from deliberate practice, you can redefine the boundaries of your potential, just as the research has shown elite performers to do.


The papers discussed in this post are:

Ericsson, K. A., Jr., Krampe, R. Th., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review (Vol. 100, Issue 3, pp. 363–406). The paper may be found here.

Ericsson, K. A. & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist49(8), 725–747. The paper may be found here.

Published in Deliberate Practice

Comments

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *